Influence Science And Practice 5th Edition By Cialdini -Test Bank
To Purchase
this Complete Test Bank with Answers Click the link Below
https://tbzuiqe.com/product/influence-science-and-practice-5th-edition-by-cialdini-test-bank/
If face any problem or
Further information contact us At tbzuiqe@gmail.com
Sample
Test
Chapter Three
Commitment and
Consistency
Teaching Commitment
and Consistency:
As with reciprocity,
consistency is valued in society. Consistency is associated with personal and intellectual
strength—rationality,
stability, honesty. Inconsistency, on the other hand, is considered an
undesirable personality
trait.
Sherman (1980) called
residents of Bloomington, Indiana and asked what they would say if they were
asked to
spend three hours
collecting money for the American Cancer Society. Many said yes.
Then, when a
representative of the American Cancer Society called a few days later, those
previously called by
Sherman volunteered at
7 times the rate of people not previously called.
Telemarketers often
begin by asking “How are you this evening?” When you say “Fine,” they respond,
“I’m glad
to hear that, because
I’m calling to ask you to make a donation to help out the unfortunate victims
of…”
Daniel Howard (1990)
tested this in a study. A telemarketer began with either “How are you feeling
this
evening?” (&
waiting for response) or “I hope you are feeling well this evening.” 33%
complied with question.
15% complied with
statement.
Another common tactic
is to begin with a small request and then move on to a larger one. This
happened to me
recently when I
received a renewal letter from a civic organization. I normally give them $35 a
year. The renewal
card had choices of
$35, $50, $100, $200, and I was surprised to find that instead of $35 being the
circled option,
$50 was circled. When
I read the letter, I noticed that the dues had not changed – still $35 a year.
But the
organization
anticipated (correctly in many cases) that their members might be willing to
give a little more, since
they’ve already
committed to giving in the past.
Another organization
that I won’t name, actually had the audacity to not even list my current
donation level on the
response card! After
debating whether to give at all, I wrote in my usual amount in the “other”
line.
The tactic of starting
with a small request and moving to a larger request is called the
“foot-in-the-door”.
Freedman and Fraser
(1966). Researcher, posing as a volunteer worker, went door-to-door in a
residential
California
neighborhood. He asked homeowners if they would allow a large public-service
billboard to be put up
on their lawns. He
showed them a picture of an attractive house with a very large, unattractive
sign reading
DRIVE CAREFULLY
obscuring the view.
What response rate do
you think he got? 17%
However, another group
of homeowners, given the same request complied at 76%! What inspired these
latter
homeowners to agree at
such a remarkable rate? Well, two weeks before, a different volunteer worker
requested
that they display a
small three inch square sign that read BE A SAFE DRIVER. Most said yes. But
then two
weeks later, they
again complied at an astounding rate.
Their second
experiment started with a request that homeowners sign a petition that favored
“keeping California
beautiful”. Of course
nearly everyone signed. Who wouldn’t want to keep their state beautiful? But
then two
weeks later, nearly
half agreed to put up the DRIVE CAREFULLY billboard even though the original
request was
on a completely
different subject!
Why? Signing the
petition changed how people viewed themselves.
Copyright © 2009
Pearson Education, Inc. Publishing as Allyn & Bacon.
21
INSTRUCTOR’S MANUAL
AND TEST BANK
Foot-in-the-Door
“What may occur is a
change in the person’s
feelings about getting
involved or taking
action. Once he has
agreed to a request, his
attitude may change,
he may become in his
own eyes, the kind of
person who does this
sort of thing, who
agrees to requests made by
strangers, who takes
action on things he
believes in, who
cooperates with good
causes.”
– Freedman and Fraser
(1966)
Foot-in-the-Door
• Start with a small
request, then move on
to a related larger
request
• Small commitments
manipulate a
person’s self-image
• Once the self-image
has been changed,
people will naturally
comply with large
requests that are
consistent with the new
self-image
Commitments that
change the self-image
should be:
• Active
• Public
• Effortful
• Freely-chosen
Here’s a vivid example
of how effective a foot-in-the-door can be. The other day, I was having lunch
with some
colleagues. One
colleague was talking about a friend from college. He noticed that the friend
had a burn of some
sort on his arm. When
asked, the friend explained it was a brand from his fraternity. And it wasn’t
the only
brand…
The colleague turned
to me and asked if social psychology had any theories as to why someone would
let people
brand him, not once
but multiple times.
As it turned out, we
were talking about that very thing in class this week. So, why would someone
allow himself
to be branded?
Well, would he have
allowed himself to be branded when he first pledged? Probably not. Instead,
people are
asked to make a series
of increasingly large commitments.
So, how do you think
this branded fraternity brother feels about his frat? He loves it! As the
research suggests, if
someone goes through a
severe initiation to join an organization (such as getting branded for a
fraternity), he or
she will become a
loyal, happy member.
Copyright © 2009
Pearson Education, Inc. Publishing as Allyn & Bacon.
22
INFLUENCE: SCIENCE AND
PRACTICE, 5E
Some effects of public
commitments:
• Circumventing
“cooling off” laws
• Increasing the
likelihood of hung juries
• Helping people lose
weight and quit
smoking
Commitments needn’t be
this large to create compliance. Just having someone write something down is
often
enough. Many car
dealerships in states with “cooling off” laws use a simple technique to prevent
customers from
backing out of
contracts. They have the customer fill out the contract.
Commitment can also create
resistance. Kerr and MacCoun (1985) showed that hung juries were significantly
more frequent when
voting took place through a show of hands rather than by secret ballot.
Weight reduction.
Quitting smoking.
By making public
commitments to lose weight or quit smoking, you are harnessing the power of the
Commitment/Consistency
principle in your favor. It’s much more difficult to go back on a public
commitment to
others than on a
private commitment to yourself. You don’t want everyone around you to think
you’re inconsistent
do you?! Going back to
the previous example of the student who likes his fraternity more than ever
after his
branding can lend some
valuable insight into increasing the effectiveness of a weight loss or smoking
cessation
program as well. Just
as a harsh initiation can lead to increased liking for a group, a more
difficult weight loss
program has been shown
to be more effective in getting people to lose weight, and keep the weight off,
even if the
increased difficulty
is a result of tasks unrelated to weight loss itself. A more difficult program
leads to a stronger
commitment to keeping
the weight off, and even more positive feelings toward losing weight.
Vulnerability to
trappings of commitments:
• Age
• Individualism
• Pride
• Self-consciousness
Research also shows
individual differences in peoples’ preference to remain consistent with prior
commitments.
One such difference is
age; people who are older tend to have a greater desire to stick to their
commitments
(Brown, Asher, &
Cialdini, 2005). Also, people with an individualistic orientation (the tendency
to value
individual achievement
as opposed to instead emphasizing the needs of the group) tend to place a
higher value on
their own prior
commitments. Therefore, those who were raised in individualistic societies such
as the U.S. or
Western Europe, as
opposed to collectivistic cultures such as Japan or China, will tend to remain
more consistent
with prior commitments
(Petrova, Cialdini, & Sills, 2007).
Similarly, those who
have a lot of personal pride or self-consciousness will tend to place more
value on their prior
commitments and remain
more consistent with them.
The last
commitment/consistency technique we’ll discuss is the low-ball.
I got hit by a
low-ball myself when I bought my first pinball machine. I had called around to
find my favorite
game (Twilight Zone).
I found one and was quoted a price of $1500. I then called a knowledgeable
friend (with a
truck) to come look at
the game with me. Once there we examined the game. It looked good. But when I
got ready
to buy it, the
attendant called the district manager and found out that they couldn’t sell it
for $1500. They wanted
$1550. I got angry,
but there I was. I had brought my friend all the way to the mall with his
truck. We had checked
the game out. And now
I wasn’t going to buy it for a measly $50? So I bought it.
Copyright © 2009
Pearson Education, Inc. Publishing as Allyn & Bacon.
23
INSTRUCTOR’S MANUAL
AND TEST BANK
The low-ball
• Begins with a good
deal (often too good)
• Then, just before
the sale, the deal
changes
• In the meantime, the
target has generated
additional
justifications
• When the original
reason is removed, the
additional
justifications remain
The low-ball can be
used for pro-social purposes. Pallak, Cook, and Sullivan (1980) wanted to
increase energy
conservation in Iowa.
An interviewer contacted residents, gave them energy-saving tips, and asked
them to try to
save fuel in the
future. They all agreed to try, but they didn’t use any less energy than a
control group who had not
been contacted.
Another group was
given the same energy-saving tips and also agreed to try to save energy. But
this group got
something extra: The
interviewer explained that their names would be published in newspaper articles
describing
them as
public-spirited, fuel-conserving citizens. This public commitment led to
substantial conservation over the
next month (12.2% gas
savings)
But then the
researchers hit them with the low-ball. The Iowans received a letter explaining
that it would not be
possible to publish
their names. Now that the incentive was removed, did their energy use go back
up? No. They
actually saved even
more energy! 15.5%.
Though the study
didn’t allow us to know exactly why this happened, one explanation is that once
the Iowans
were promised
newspaper publicity, they began conserving energy. Then they started generating
other reasons
why they were
conserving energy. When the original reason was taken away, these other reasons
remained.
Though perhaps not
entirely ethical, the low-ball can even be used by consumers (normally the
targets of such
techniques). A good
friend of mine developed the following technique. He goes into a pawn shop and
finds
something he wants. He
asks the price but after checking his pocket, he finds that he doesn’t have
quite enough
money. He gives the
storeowner all the cash he has and leaves to get more. He returns later,
morose, and explains
that he can’t get any
more cash. Often, the owner grumbles and sells him the item for the lower
price.
Interestingly, my
friend can accurately predict when this will work and when it won’t. It doesn’t
work if the
owner puts the money
aside behind the counter. But if he puts it in his pocket, my friend’s got it
made.
Defense
• Body signals
• Stomach signs
– Listen to your gut
– Tell requester
exactly what they did
• “Heart of Hearts”
– Ask self if you’d
make the same
commitments if you had
it to do over
again, and go with
first feeling
Copyright © 2009
Pearson Education, Inc. Publishing as Allyn & Bacon.
24
INFLUENCE: SCIENCE AND
PRACTICE, 5E
The best ways to
defend yourself against the weapon of commitment/consistency involve paying
close attention to
body signs.
Commitment/consistency works by trapping you in your own previous commitments,
and this is often
accompanied by an
uncomfortable tightening in your gut as the trap begins to spring. The key is
to trust this
feeling and use it to
your advantage. If you feel like you’re being taken, there’s a good chance that
you are. At this
point, listen to your
gut and tell the requester exactly what they’ve done. They’re sure to back off.
However, we are very
good at fooling ourselves into believing what is consistent with our prior
beliefs. We can
come up with endless
reasons to justify even the most foolish of behaviors. Despite this, there is
still a way to reevaluate
the situation: Ask
yourself if, knowing what you know now, you would make the same commitments
again, and listen pay
close attention to the first hint of an answer that comes into being before all
the excuses and
rationalizations can
take over.
Over the last week or
two, we’ve discussed two weapons of influence: reciprocity and
commitment/consistency.
These aren’t just
abstract theories, though. They’re effective and usable techniques. So, I’d
like everyone to divide
into groups and
consider some of the applications of the techniques we’ve been discussing.
For the following
questions, imagine that
you’re in a class in
which the professor gives
essay tests. On the
last test, you felt your
essays were worth an
A, but when the tests
were returned, you
received a B+. You
decide to talk to the
professor about changing
your grade.
How could you use
reciprocity to help
convince the professor
to change your grade?
How could you use
commitment/consistency
to help convince the
professor
I’ve found that
students respond well to this exercise. By violating the taboo about discussing
such matters
in class, students
become engaged. In addition, the solutions the students develop inevitably
raise important
questions about the
ethics involved in invoking the weapons of influence.
Copyright © 2009
Pearson Education, Inc. Publishing as Allyn & Bacon.
25
INSTRUCTOR’S MANUAL
AND TEST BANK
Testing Commitment and
Consistency:
3.1 Someone is
especially vulnerable to the Commitment and Consistency principle if he/she is
(a) young
(b) raised in a
Western society
(c) coerced into
making the initial commitment
(d) a and b only
(e) all of the above
ANSWER: B
3.2 An experiment done
at a horsetrack measured bettors’ confidence that their horse would
win. Half of the
bettors were approached immediately before laying down their bets and
the other half were
approached immediately after. What was found?
(a) Those approached
after their bets were significantly more confident than those
approached before.
(b) Those approached
before their bets were significantly more confident than those
approached after.
(c) Both groups were
equally confident.
(d) None of the above.
ANSWER: A
3.3 Which of the
following alternatives best describes the explanation given in Influence for
our strong desire to
be and look consistent?
(a) Consistency is
adaptive; that is, most of the time we are better off with a consistent
approach to things.
(b) Most people in
desire to remain consistent with themselves.
(c) Most people see
inconsistency as an undesirable trait in others.
(d) All of the above.
(e) Only a and c.
ANSWER: D
Copyright © 2009
Pearson Education, Inc. Publishing as Allyn & Bacon.
26
INFLUENCE: SCIENCE AND
PRACTICE, 5E
3.4 Automatic,
unthinking consistency is a dangerous thing. Yet, in Influence, it was stated
that many times we
find it attractive. Why?
(a) It offers one
shortcut to use in deciding how to behave when we don’t have the time to
examine all the
evidence.
(b) Because we have a
deep-seated need for occasional danger.
(c) Because even
unthinking consistency is preferable to emotional responding.
(d) None of the above.
ANSWER: A
3.5 In Influence
several factors were discussed that increased the ability of a commitment to
produce changes in a
person’s self-image and future action. Which of the following is not
such a factor? When
the commitment is:
(a) active.
(b) effortful.
(c) public.
(d) perceived as
internally motivated.
(e) none of the above;
they are all such factors.
ANSWER: E
3.6 Which of the
following is true regarding the foot-in-the-door technique?
(a) People are more
likely to agree to a second request after denying in initial, smaller
request.
(b) People are more
likely to agree to a second request after agreeing to an initial, smaller
request.
(c) People are more
likely to agree to a second request after denying in initial, larger
request.
(d) People are more
likely to agree to a second request after agreeing to an initial, larger
request.
ANSWER: B
3.7 Why do written
commitments tend to be effective?
(a) They are active.
(b) They are difficult
to deny once made.
(c) They influence
others to believe that the writer truly believes what was written.
(d) All of the above.
(e) None of the above.
ANSWER: D
Copyright © 2009
Pearson Education, Inc. Publishing as Allyn & Bacon.
27
INSTRUCTOR’S MANUAL
AND TEST BANK
3.8 In a study by
Jones and Harris, subjects were shown an essay that was favorable to Fidel
Castro. Half were told
that the essay’s author had been assigned to write the pro-Castro
essay, whereas the
other half were told that the author had chosen to write the pro-Castro
essay. All subjects
were then asked to estimate the true attitude of the author toward
Castro. What happened?
(a) Only when the
author was seen as having chosen to write the essay was he rated as
liking Castro.
(b) Only when the
author was seen as required to write the essay was he rated as liking
Castro.
(c) The author was
rated as liking Castro both when he was seen as having chosen and as
having been assigned
the essay.
(d) The author was
only rated as liking Castro if the subject making the rating liked Castro.
ANSWER: C
3.9 In a study by
Deutsch and Gerard, subjects committed themselves to some initial
judgments, privately,
or not at all. Later they received information that their initial
judgements may have
been mistaken. They were then given the opportunity to change
those first judgments.
What happened?
(a) Both public and
private commitment produced equal loyalty to the initial judgments.
(b) Both public and
private commitment produced loyalty, but the public commitment
produced more.
(c) Both public and
private commitment produced loyalty, but the private commitment
produced more.
(d) None of the above.
ANSWER: B
3.10 The study by
Aronson and Mills in which some college women were made to go through
an embarrassing
initiation ceremony in order to gain access to a discussion group:
(a) found those who
had the severe initiation disliked the discussion group as a result.
(b) found those who
had the severe initiation liked the discussion group more as a result.
(c) has implications
for understanding the actions of such diverse groups as college
fraternities and
primitive societies.
(d) only a and c.
(e) only b and c.
ANSWER: E
Copyright © 2009
Pearson Education, Inc. Publishing as Allyn & Bacon.
28
INFLUENCE: SCIENCE AND
PRACTICE, 5E
3.11 Jonathan Freedman
conducted a study in which students at an all-boys school were
threatened with mild
or severe punishment for playing with a “forbidden toy”.
Approximately 6 weeks
later, were given another opportunity to play with the “forbidden
toy.” Which of the
following is true regarding the boys’ behavior?
(a) Most of those who
were threatened with mild punishment tended to play with the toy
the first time, but
few played with it the second time.
(b) Most of those who
were threatened with mild punishment tended to play with the toy
the second time, but
few played with it the first time.
(c) Most of those who
were threatened with severe punishment tended to play with the toy
the first time, but
few played with it the second time.
(d) Most of those who
were threatened with severe punishment tended to play with the toy
the second time, but
few played with it the first time.
ANSWER: D
3.12 A research
project by Pallak and his associates demonstrated the effectiveness of the
lowball
compliance technique
often used by car dealers. In the Pallak work, Iowa homeowners
were promised
newspaper publicity if they conserved energy, which led to energy savings
initially. A month later
they were told that they would not get the publicity after all. What
happened?
(a) Canceling the
publicity had no effect, they continued to save energy at the same rate as
they did after first
being told about the publicity.
(b) Canceling the
publicity caused them to revert to their old energy consumption levels.
(c) Canceling the
publicity resulted in even greater energy savings.
(d) Only a and b.
(e) Only a and c.
ANSWER: E
3.13 Which of the
following defenses are suggested in Influence to combat use of the weapon of
Commitment and
Consistency?
(a) Always take time
when performing a behavior to stop and evaluate whether behaving
consistently with
prior actions is the best decision.
(b) Deliberate
carefully about whether you would make the same past decisions if you
could go back in time
knowing what you know now.
(c) Pay attention to
your gut feelings and when you feel like you’re being trapped by your
own commitments, share
that with whomever is trapping you.
(d) All of the above.
ANSWER: C
Copyright © 2009
Pearson Education, Inc. Publishing as Allyn & Bacon.
29
INSTRUCTOR’S MANUAL
AND TEST BANK
3.14 Which of the
following would be the best way to ensure that people do not discuss a survey
they completed until
the end of the semester?
(a) “Do not discuss
the survey until the end of the semester.”
(b) “Please do not
discuss the survey until the end of the semester.”
(c) “Do you agree not
to discuss the survey until the end of the semester?”
(d) “I ask that you
please do not discuss the survey until the end of the semester.”
ANSWER: C
3.15 Which of the
following statements is true about commitments?
(a) Once a person
makes an initial commitment, he is more likely to agree to future
requests that are in
keeping with the prior commitment.
(b) If a person makes
a commitment but later finds out that the original reason for making
the commitment was not
valid, the original commitment will not affect his later behavior.
(c) An initial
commitment must be very large if it is going to affect later behavior.
(d) All of the above
ANSWER: A
Copyright © 2009
Pearson Education, Inc. Publishing as Allyn & Bacon.
30
INFLUENCE: SCIENCE AND
PRACTICE, 5E
Chapter Four
Social Proof
Teaching Social Proof:
For social proof, I
begin by showing the History Channel documentary “History’s Mysteries:
Silent Witnesses, The
Kitty Genovese Murder” (part AAE-42628, orderable on
http://www.historychannel.com).
It is, in my opinion, a somewhat flawed documentary, spending
relatively little time
on the social psychological explanations for the inaction of the bystanders. It
does, however, offer a
disturbing portrayal of a disturbing historical event.
When the videotape is
finished, I assess the class’ reaction. If students are upset (which sometimes
happens
after viewing the documentary)
we begin discussing the documentary immediately. Otherwise, I start with
the theory and
research below, maintaining flexibility when the class wishes to discuss or
elaborate on
particular points.
This is, in my
opinion, a somewhat flawed documentary—particularly in the almost complete
exclusion of the
social psychological
explanation of the incident. The producers knew about the research. They
mentioned
(although
mispronounced) the original researchers: Latané & Darley.
Why did 38 witnesses
do nothing?
• Common explanations
at the time:
– People, particularly
city dwellers, are
apathetic
– People are selfish,
insensitive,
unfeeling, and
indifferent
• Explanations of
social psychologists
– Diffusion of
responsibility
– Witnesses saw others
doing nothing
and inferred that they
should do nothing
Many commentators
emphasized that no action was taken even though 38 witnesses were present.
Two social
psychologists, Bibb Latané and John Darley, suggested, instead, that no action
was taken because 38
witnesses were
present.
Two reasons: (a)
diffusion of responsibility, (b) because we want to appear poised and cool in
front of others.
When we look around,
we see others failing to act or even look upset. So there must be nothing
wrong.
Bystander intervention
• Darley and Latané
(1968): Confederate
had an epileptic
seizure.
– One bystander:
– Five bystanders:
• Latané and Darley
(1968): Smoke
seeping out from under
a door
– Lone participant:
– Three participants:
– One participant, two
passive
confederates:
Copyright © 2009
Pearson Education, Inc. Publishing as Allyn & Bacon.
31
INSTRUCTOR’S MANUAL
AND TEST BANK
For both of these
studies, I write in the results as we discuss them. Darley & Latané, 1968:
RA had an
epileptic seizure. 85%
helped with only one bystander. 31% when 5 bystanders were present. Latané
& Darley,
1968: 75% of lone
individuals who see smoke seeping from under a door report it. Three person
groups reported it
only 38% of the time.
When a participant was in a three-person group with two passive confederates,
the
participant reported
the smoke only 10% of the time.
Bystander intervention
• Very high levels of
helping (90-100%)
when need is clear
• In ambiguous
situations, more help with
a lone bystander
• Pluralistic
ignorance greatest in groups
of strangers. Worst in
cities:
– loud and distracting
– multiple witnesses
more likely
– other witnesses are
likely to be
strangers
Research has shown
high levels of helping (90-100%) when the need is clear – whether witnesses see
it singly or
in groups. In
ambiguous situations, there is often more help with a lone bystander.
Pluralistic ignorance is greatest
in groups of
strangers. Worst in cities: cities are loud and distracting, multiple witnesses
are more likely, and
others are likely to
be strangers. Perhaps these offer a better explanation for the bystander
inaction during the
Kitty Genovese murder.
Social Proof:
We view a behavior as
correct in a given
situation to the
degree that we see others
performing it.
As with other
influence principles, usually this works well. Usually if a lot of people are
doing something, it’s the
right thing to do. I
then ask the class to brainstorm about situations in which they’ve used others’
behavior
as a guide for theirs.
We often use the
behavior of others as a
guide when we are
unsure of what to do.
What are three
situations in which you’ve
used social proof to
determine the correct
behavior?
1)
2)
3)
Later today, we’ll
listen to some segments of
the National Public
Radio fund drive. While
listening to these segments,
please write
down the uses of
social proof that you hear:
Copyright © 2009
Pearson Education, Inc. Publishing as Allyn & Bacon.
32
INFLUENCE: SCIENCE AND
PRACTICE, 5E
Social influence via
social proof
• Laugh tracks
• Salting
• “Fastest-growing”
“Largest-selling”
“Number one”
• Overcoming fears
– Bandura, Grusec, and
Menlove (1967)
– Bandura and Menlove
(1968)
Laugh tracks: when a
show has a laugh track people laugh longer and more often, and they rate the
material as
funnier (Provine,
2000). Canned laughter is most effective for poor jokes (Nosanchuk &
Lightstone, 1974).
Salting: Bartenders
salt tip jars, church ushers salt collection plates.
Products are labeled
“fastest-growing”, “largest-selling”, or “Number one.” It is not uncommon to,
shortly after a
new Year or season
begins, encounter a movie advertisement claiming the movie is “the number 1
romantic
comedy of the Year”,
or “the number one thriller of the Summer.” While this initially sounds
impressive, closer
inspection often
reveals that the romantic comedy or thriller in question is the only new movie
of that genre so far
that year or season.
Of course this qualifies these movies for last place as much as it does first,
but advertisers who
know the power of
social proof are happy to overlook that.
Social proof helps
people overcome fears. (Bandura, Grusec, & Menlove, 1967). Children who
were terrified of
dogs watched a little
boy playing happily with a dog for 20 minutes each day. After 4 days, 2/3 were
willing to
climb into a playpen with
a dog and remain confined while everyone else left the room. The effect did not
diminish a month
later. A follow-up study also showed the effect when the children watched a
film of a boy
playing with a dog
(Bandura & Menlove, 1968). It was most effective when the film showed a
variety of other
children interacting
with their dogs.
When we are unsure of
ourselves, when the
situation is unclear
or ambiguous, when
uncertainty reigns, we
are most likely to look
to and accept the
actions of others as correct
(Tesser, Campbell,
& Mickler, 1983).
When is social proof
most common? When we are unsure…
But the people we are
examining are probably examining us, so appearances can be deceiving.
Amplifiers of Social
Proof
• Uncertainty
– Advice for helping
• Similarity
• Many others
• Collectivistic
cultures
Advice: If you are in
an emergency, point to individual people, make them aware of the emergency,
make them
individually
responsible for actions, and instruct them on what to do (“You in the green
shirt, call 911!”).
Copyright © 2009
Pearson Education, Inc. Publishing as Allyn & Bacon.
33
INSTRUCTOR’S MANUAL
AND TEST BANK
We are more likely to
use another’s behavior as a model for our own behavior when the other is
similar to
ourselves. Hence the
frequent use of person on the street interviews in advertisements.
Also, the number of
others performing a behavior has a strong effect on the influence of this
behavior. This, of
course, makes sense.
If everyone else is doing it, then it must be the right thing to do, and if
more people are doing
it, it seems to be an
even better cue.
Finally, the culture
in which someone was raised can affect the strength with which the principle of
social proof
can move behavior. In
collectivistic cultures such as Japan or China, there is more emphasis on
fitting in with the
group as opposed to an
emphasis on standing apart like in individualistic cultures like the U.S. or
Western Europe.
As a result, those who
are raised in collectivistic cultures tend to be more easily swayed by social
proof.
I then have the class
listen to a 2-3 minute segment of a National Public Radio fund drive, which
contains a
number of instances of
social proof (telephones ringing, names of people who have pledged, etc.)
Defense
• Stay alert for faked
evidence
• Stay alert for
social proof via social
proof
– Social Proof
snowball effect
There are two main
actions to take in order to defend yourself against being misguided by the
principle of social
proof, and each one
just requires you to periodically take a quick peek at your surroundings.
The first is to keep
an eye out for falsified evidence of social proof. If the evidence that
something is funny is a
laugh track, or the
evidence that something is popular is an actor playing a random person on the
street, it’s time to
think more carefully
and really consider the evidence at hand. Luckily, this kind of falsified
evidence is fairly easy
to identify most of
the time.
The other action is to
keep an eye out for the possibility that the social proof you see may be the
result of a snowball
effect. That is, it’s
okay to use the crowd as a valuable information source if they are all doing
something because
they have reason for
doing it, but it’s also possible that everyone is performing a certain behavior
just because
everyone else is doing
it. In this case, again, you should think more carefully about the evidence you
and everone
else actually has
about the situation at hand.
Copyright © 2009
Pearson Education, Inc. Publishing as Allyn & Bacon.
34
INFLUENCE: SCIENCE AND
PRACTICE, 5E
Testing Social Proof:
4.1 Someone is likely
to be more vulnerable to the social proof principle if he/she
(a) is similar to
those who are providing the social proof evidence
(b) was raised in a
collectivistic society
(c) is uncertain of the
best course of action
(d) b and c only
(e) All of the above.
ANSWER: E
4.2 What has the
research on the principle of social proof shown?
(a) It works best when
the proof is provided by the actions of many other people.
(b) It isn’t strong
enough to overcome deep-seated fears or phobias.
(c) It works on adults
but not children.
(d) Live
demonstrations of others’ behavior are necessary for the effect to work; film
clips
don’t produce good
results.
ANSWER: A
4.3 In the
Chicago-based doomsday cult studied by Festinger, Riecken, and Schacter, what
happened after it
became clear that the end of the world was not going to occur?
(a) The group
disbanded after members saw each other show disappointment.
(b) The group members
remained highly committed to the group.
(c) The groups ceased
in trying to gain publicity.
(d) B and C only
ANSWER: B
4.4 The concept of
pluralistic ignorance:
(a) defines a
situation in which each person viewing a possible emergency decides that
since nobody appears
concerned, nothing is wrong.
(b) helps explain the
results of the bystander intervention studies done by Latane and
Darley.
(c) helps explain the
tendency of Jonestown residents to wait calmly in line to take poison.
(d) all of the above.
(e) only a and c.
ANSWER: D
Copyright © 2009
Pearson Education, Inc. Publishing as Allyn & Bacon.
35
INSTRUCTOR’S MANUAL
AND TEST BANK
4.5 Which of the
following maximize(s) the influence of the principle of social proof?
(a) Uncertainty
regarding the situation.
(b) A feeling of
obligation toward the others in the situation.
(c) A feeling of
similarity to the others in the situation.
(d) Only b and c.
(e) Only a and c.
ANSWER: E
4.6 Which of the
following is false regarding helping in an emergency?
(a) When a bystander
is surrounded by passive others, he or she is unlikely to take action.
(b) When it is clear
that an emergency exists, bystanders usually help.
(c) Whether the
situation is clear or unclear, help is more likely when one bystander (rather
than a group) is
present.
(d) In an unclear situation,
a group of strangers is less likely to produce help than a group
of friends.
ANSWER: C
4.7 Which of the
following natural characteristics of urban environments are likely to lead to
decreased bystander
aid?
(a) confusion.
(b) populousness.
(c) low levels of
acquaintanceship.
(d) a and b only.
(e) all of the above.
ANSWER: E
4.8 Each of the
following increase the chance that helping will occur in a situation EXCEPT
(a) it is clear that
help is needed
(b) it is clear who is
responsible for helping
(c) it is clear what
needs to be done to help
(d) None of the above
(all will increase the chance of helping)
ANSWER: D
Copyright © 2009
Pearson Education, Inc. Publishing as Allyn & Bacon.
36
INFLUENCE: SCIENCE AND
PRACTICE, 5E
4.9 The strange
statistic showing that front page suicide stories lead to increases in airplane
and
automobile fatalities
was argued in Influence to be best explained by which of the
following
interpretations?
(a) Bereavement.
(b) Secret Imitation
Suicide.
(c) Similar Social
Conditions.
(d) None of the above.
ANSWER: B
4.10 Which of the
following is not true of the relationship between front page suicides and
subsequent air and
traffic deaths?
(a) The wider the
publicity given the suicide story, the greater the increase in subsequent
deaths.
(b) The relationship
holds for noncommercial air fatalities but not for commercial (i.e.,
airline) fatalities.
(c) If the story
concerns a single suicide, then there is an increase only in single-person air
and traffic
fatalities.
(d) If the story
concerns a suicide plus murder incident, then there is an increase only in
multiple-person air
and traffic deaths.
(e) The age of the
single-person suicide story victim is correlated with the age of the
victims of subsequent
single-person car wrecks.
ANSWER: B
4.11 Someone is more
likely to follow the lead of others who are
(a) large in number
(b) performing a
behavior with some dissenters present
(c) uncertain of their
actions
(d) All of the above
ANSWER: A
4.12 Which of the
following is false about laugh tracks?
(a) Laugh tracks have
a greater effect on poor jokes than good ones.
(b) Audiences like
laugh tracks.
(c) Audiences laugh
more when laugh tracks are present.
(d) Audiences rate
shows as funnier when they contain laugh tracks.
ANSWER: B
Copyright © 2009
Pearson Education, Inc. Publishing as Allyn & Bacon.
37
INSTRUCTOR’S MANUAL
AND TEST BANK
4.13 According to
Influence, fast food ads are effective in part because they
(a) change people’s
attitudes toward the food
(b) allow viewers to
see diverse groups of people eating fast food
(c) make the act of
eating fast food seem more common
(d) All of the above
ANSWER: C
4.14 Which of the
following statements is true about social proof?
(a) Using social proof
usually leads us to the wrong behavior.
(b) Social proof can
help to explain the inaction of the bystanders to the Kitty Genovese
murder.
(c) We often follow
social evidence, even when it is clearly fake.
(d) Both b and c.
(e) All of the above.
ANSWER: D
4.15 According to
Influence, to defend oneself against the inappropriate use of social proof, we
should
(a) stay vigilant for
plainly counterfeit social evidence.
(b) never look to
others to figure out what to do.
(c) look only to
dissimilar others for evidence of correct behavior.
(d) None of the above.
ANSWER: A
Copyright
Comments
Post a Comment